The Oxford Handbook of Gossip and Reputation

Here are some excerpts from this 2019 book:

* GOSSIP and reputation are core processes in all human societies. Consequently, humans invest a great amount of effort to keep track of others’ reputation and to effectively manage their own. This is especially true in the contemporary world. New technologies increased the number of potential partners and interactions and changed the way we deal with information about others. Reputation management companies and specialists are no longer employed only by movie stars and firms’ CEOs, but these services are required by more and more people. According to Forbes, 82% of executive recruiters report that positive information found online can improve a candidate’s job prospects, but also that firms risk losing more than 20% of business when potential customers find a negative review on their first page of search results. In the offline world, positive or negative reputations result from gossip, which is a primary source of information about others, and it is also a very popular activity, widespread across time and culture.

Gossip and reputation are multifaceted social phenomena. As theoretical constructs, they share several characteristics that pose a challenge for attempts to get a grip on them. First and foremost, both are part of a triadic relation, in which at least three types of actors “engage” with each other. Gossiping requires somebody (a sender) conveying information about a third party (object) to somebody else (receiver); having a reputation implies that the information we receive about someone’s presumed qualities has been generated by somebody else. It involves at least three relational acts: an act of attribution, in which someone attaches an (evaluative) quality to someone else (e.g., Vaidyanathan, Khalsa, & Ecklund, 2016); an act of sharing, in which this attribution is communicated (Hallett, Harger, & Eder, 2009) to others; and an act of perception in which this attribution is recognized and understood as such by a receiver (p. 2) (Kuttler, Parker, & LaGreca, 2002). In the case of gossip, an additional condition is that it requires the absence of the third party, that is, secrecy at the moment of transmission. Any attempt to systematically observe these phenomena in real-life or in the lab will have to find a way to capture this combination of attribution, communication, and perception in triadic structures. In addition to psychological complexity, the triadic and relational aspects of gossip and reputation also come with structural complexity. For example, for the individuals involved to disclose sensitive or evaluative third-party information, power differences matter (Ellwardt, Wittek, & Wielers, 2012; Jeuken et al., 2015).

Second, in most societies the act of gossiping, but also of strategically “managing” one’s own reputation or “damaging” the reputation of others, tends to be normatively regulated and morally laden (Alfano & Robinson, 2017; Bertolotti & Magnani, 2014; Fernandes, Kapoor, & Karandikar, 2017; Peters & Kashima, 2015; Radzik, 2016). The discourse on gossip illustrates this nicely, since for each negative view on gossip, there is a positive one. According to the philosopher Henry Lanz: “In gossip we are pleased to discuss other people’s faults, seldom their merits. We thus seem to enjoy evil for evil’s sake. For we are pleased by faults and errors. We are content to see them endure and grow. We are eager to augment their number and to exaggerate their importance” (Lanz, 1936, p. 494). In contrast, Robin Dunbar, who posited that gossip could have played a major role in the evolution of language, believes that gossip is “the central plank on which human sociality
is founded” (2004, p. 109). Similarly, whereas many emphasize effective reputation management as the key to success for individuals and firms, others point to the “dangerous art of impression management.”

Third, the moral connotation of both phenomena is related to the fact that they require agency of those involved and therefore allow strategic behavior. Individuals may deliberately spread lies about others (Seki & Nakamaru, 2016), or they may attempt to manipulate the image others have about them. Although gossip has been described as “cheap talk” (Coleman, 1990), it is evident that not everybody will share everything about any third party with anyone else: selective disclosure can be of tremendous strategic value for furthering the interests of oneself or one’s group (Burt, 1992). Consequently, assessing the veracity of gossip (Hess & Hagen, 2006; Kuttler, Parker, & La Greca, 2002) becomes a challenge of its own.

Fourth, judging from the evidence that has been compiled so far, gossip and reputation are truly multipurpose social phenomena. As the chapters in this Handbook also demonstrate, the list of their potential “functions” for individuals and groups is impressive, ranging from their impact on emotions and the fulfilment of basic human needs to the cohesion of groups and human sociality in general.

Fifth, the wide-ranging impact of gossip and reputation may stem from the pivotal role they have played in human evolution (e.g., Massar, Buunk, & Rempt, 2012). Their evolutionary base may explain not only the strong emotional and neurophysiological reactions they can trigger (Anderson et al., 2011; Brondino, Fusar-Poli, & Politi, 2017; Peng et al.,
2015), but also account for the distinct variations in their behavioral base and impact between the sexes or along social hierarchies.

Finally, whereas recent research provides evidence for cross-cultural measurement invariance for (workplace) gossip (Brady, Brown & Liang, 2017) and for reputation as a “universal currency for human social interactions” (Milinski, 2016), the antecedents, processes, and consequences of gossip and reputation are highly context dependent. This holds not only for differences across cultures (Henrich et al., 2006; Marlowe et al., 2008), but also across other kinds of social collectives. For example, the incidence, content, form, and function of gossip and reputation may vary depending on the social-structural environment, such as the kind and degree of (inter)dependence in organizations or communities or the socioeconomic position of those involved.

Despite their importance in social life, academic interest in gossip and reputation has developed relatively recently. In 1993, Bromley wrote, “Reputation is a phenomenon of considerable social and scientific importance, but the interest shown in it by writers and by ordinary people has not been paralleled by an equivalent degree of interest shown by social and behavioural scientists” (Bromley, 1993, p. 8). A similar concern was shared by Goodman (1994), who wrote in the introduction to his edited volume on gossip that “until recently, philosophers and social scientists have paid scant attention to gossip” (p. 1). Still in 2004, Wert and Salovey wrote in their introduction to the Special Issue on Gossip
published by the Review of General Psychology that “Gossip matters to all things social, yet social scientists have been slow to pursue its secrets” (p. 76).

Gossip, Internet-Based Reputation Systems, and Governance

The eBay electronic market provides one of the most interesting examples of Internetbased reputation systems, and it is also the most widely researched. Founded in 1995, today it has over 160 million active users around the world, generating close to one billion yearly listings. One key of its success is the fact that both sellers and buyers might write an
assessment, or “feedback”, on each other, which can be positive, neutral, or negative. The percentage of positive feedbacks received in the previous 12 months forms an index of reputation of sort. In a situation where otherwise there would be ample room for cheating, this feature provides incentives to behave honestly, to be efficient, and to invest in quality.
Similar mechanisms are in place in many electronic markets, and all Internet-based reputation systems share some of the key characteristics of eBay. Such Internet-based systems serve as tools to process reputationally relevant information in situations where traditional word-of-mouth would not work, because of the impersonal nature of the relationship among
participants who are typically geographically scattered.

* First, according to a point of view originating in Dunbar (1996), “gossip [is] a mechanism for bonding social groups, tracing these origins back to social grooming among primates” (Dunbar, 2004). According to Baumeister et al. (2004), gossip “can convey valuable information to the hearer about culture and society” and it spurs cultural learning, and several studies (for example, Gottman and Mettetal, 1986) sustain that gossip serves to promote group solidarity. In revealing “personal information about the gossiper”, gossip “communicates to the listener that he or she is trusted” (Bosson et al., 2006) and helps “cement and maintain social bonds” (Baumeister et al, 2004). Also, (negative) gossip ties persons together by providing opportunities for downward social comparisons, which, in turn ”can boost self esteem”. Bosson et al. (2006) (referring to Wert and Salovey, 2004) note that “by gossiping with a potential friend about her dislike of a third person, the gossiper signals to the gossipee that she considers him an in-group member, which should promote self-esteem and grease the wheels of their friendship.”

In fact, gossip might grease human interactions also precisely because it is perceived to be ethically wrong. Ego pays a cost in case alter exposes him as a gossiper, so that gossip might function as a bond of trust between ego and alter. Alter shares the moral blame with ego, to the extent that he approvingly accepts to hear the gossip, and even more so when, as it
often happens, roles are interchanged in a “gossiping session”. Willingly assuming the moral cost of gossiping, and the possibility of reciprocally exposing each other to the social stigma which accompanies such activity, facilitates cooperation by bonding ego and alter. For all these reasons, even negative gossip, far from having the merely destructive role that it is often assumed, might actually positively affect organizational output, and encourage cooperation beyond the direct effects of the reputational information that it disseminates.

Posted in Gossip | Comments Off on The Oxford Handbook of Gossip and Reputation

The Rise & Fall Of Jordan Peterson (12-24-20)

00:00 The Rise of Jordan Peterson (2019)
12:20 Jordan on the Jews
21:00 Jordan’s health problems, https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Jordan_Peterson#Health_problems
1:12:40 Ed Dutton and Richard Spencer’s podcast, https://www.spreaker.com/show/the-mcspencer-group
1:14:00 IQ Denial
1:26:00 Geographic chauvinism, Los Angeles vs San Francisco
1:38:00 The Ripper documentary, https://decider.com/2020/12/16/the-ripper-netflix-review/
2:11:00 EVERYTHING THE ALT-RIGHT KNOWS IS WRONG (AND ALWAYS HAS BEEN), https://affirmativeright.blogspot.com/2020/12/everything-alt-right-knows-is-wrong-and.html
2:19:40 The AR Optics debate, https://www.pscp.tv/w/1kvJpoLPeAPGE

Posted in Jordan Peterson | Comments Off on The Rise & Fall Of Jordan Peterson (12-24-20)

Not only Trolls are Trolling the Internet: A study on dark personality traits, online environment, and commentary styles

From this new paper:

* These are “Trolls” (their motive is to seek attention/revenge and are suggested to have person-ality traits associated with Sadism, low self-esteem, low conscience, and a low moral compass), “Lurkers” (they visit various forums but refrain from writing messages or comments), and “Posters” (they use forums to reveal per-sonal information that often relates to sex life, sexuality, fantasies, family problems, and personal problems, varying in motives).

* Dark traits can predict what kind of approach and values a person has towards others… The first of the three traits is “Machiavellianism” (Mach), characterized by often having a cynical worldview and by striving for power, money and status; they often are cold and pragmatic, and use false play and manipulation. They can be charming, cun-ning, strategic, enthusiastic and deficient in morals. The sec-ond of these traits is “Narcissism” (Narc), characterized by seeking attention and admiration, by being arrogant and ex-hibitionistic, and by striving for power and leadership. They often feel superior, dominant and self-righteous. They can be charismatic, productive and inspiring. Thirdly, “Psy-chopathy” (Psych), characterized by often being cold, and by being manipulative, and impulsive. They often have a low degree of empathy and little feelings of guilt… Studies have also suggested that Sadism should be included in the set of dark traits… Apart from the first three traits, Sadism can predict insensitivity to the suffering of others, and can also predict insensitivity when to “strike back” when threatened. Sadism also has shown a unique relation to Internet Trolling and to sexuality. The unique aspect of Sadism is that the individual enjoys the suffering of others.

The anonymity aspect of the online environment is reported to affect the behavior on internet (Seigfried-Spellar & Lank-ford, 2018). This could be due to de-individuation processes (Demetriou & Silke, 2003), inducing a state where individuals feel anonymous among many people. Postmes and Spear (1998) claim that de-individuation must not necessarily lead to unethical behavior on the Internet. However, de-individuation may lead to online disinhibition effects, meaning that in anonymity, communication is more uninhibited than in a face-to-face communication (Clark-Gor-don, Bowman, Goodboy, & Wright, 2019). Measures of Online Environment (OE) can be de-scribed with three subcategories (Ritter, 2014). One is “Acceptability” (relates to the hegemonic culture of the forum. Individuals who score high on OE-acceptability may feel it is okay to express themselves with both prejudice and sexist comments). A second is “Aloneness” (encourages harassment, as the OE is lacking social codes, creating inhibitory effects. Individuals do not feel they need to follow ordinary social norms and can write whatever they want). The third is “Anonymity” (neutralizes status disparities and makes individuals feel invisible while also reducing personal responsibility. Individuals participate in more risky behavior and treat people any way they want since no one knows who they are).

…The results showed that particularly Trolling (malicious posting) had significant correlations with almost all four dark personality traits. Controlling for dark personality traits, Sadism and Machiavellianism remained as the strongest predictors for Trolling. This is in line with Buckels et al.’s (2014) results that also showed a strong correlation with the Internet behavior style, and it provides more evidence of the importance of the role of especially dark personality traits. Not surprisingly, it seems that hard-minded people post more malicious content. Similarly, two of the dark personality traits, Sadism and Psychopathy, co-varied strongly with Posting (regular reading and posting on forums). Interestingly, it seems that hard-minded people also post more in conventional styles.

When the Online Environment was investigated as a mediator in these relationships, only trivial effects were found. This means that personality behind posting behaviors generally cannot be explained by how people see or feel or perceive their online environment on Internet forums. The one exception was Acceptability, which describes tolerance to harmful and socially unacceptable behavior on the Internet (Ritter, 2014), which had a small mediating effect explaining Psychopathy and conventional Posting. This study makes several contributions to research on individual behavior on the Internet. First, this study shows that an individual’s behavior on the Internet can be predicted by dark personality traits. The fact that Trolling is related to the dark traits is already known, but what is of interest is that these dark traits (especially Sadism and Psychopathy) are also represented in the regular Posters. Second, the present study also shows in an exploratory way that Online Environment cannot explain the relationship between the dark traits and the posting behavior. It seems that personality traits are “all it takes” for Internet commentary styles.

Posted in Internet | Comments Off on Not only Trolls are Trolling the Internet: A study on dark personality traits, online environment, and commentary styles

Who Made the Vaccine Possible? Not WHO

Graham Allison writes in the WSJ:

…companies like Germany-based BioNTech, its Boston-based competitor Moderna, and the pharmaceutical giant Pfizer have also been racing for a pot of gold at the end of this rainbow. There would be no Covid-19 vaccine today had there been no venture capitalists prepared to invest before a product or profit was visible, no corporate leadership willing to double down with the companies’ own money in the spring to fund a crash effort to produce a vaccine by year-end, and no researchers pursuing a dream about mRNA as an unprecedented route for vaccines.

Second is Operation Warp Speed. Had Mr. Trump not created the initiative, appointed as its leader a man who knows the vaccine development world, and given him license to spend $10 billion outside normal contracting procedures, Covid-19 vaccines would still be only works in progress. Even after they were finally approved, the vaccines’ distribution could have been long delayed. Imagine a world in which Mr. Trump had not appointed as deputy head of the operation a general who knows logistics and had the authority to write contracts with FedEx and UPS to book space on their airplanes and in their network of distribution centers.

So as Americans now look forward to getting vaccinated and resuming our normal lives, we should pause to give thanks to a remarkable group of scientists and entrepreneurs whose capitalism-fed competitive drive pushed them to venture into the unknown—for fortune and fame. And to a deeply flawed, often dysfunctional disrupter in chief who in this case certainly did a good thing.

Posted in Covid | Comments Off on Who Made the Vaccine Possible? Not WHO

FEE: We Had the Vaccine from the Start—You Just Weren’t Allowed to Take It

00:00 Moderna’s highly effective vaccine was available in January, https://fee.org/articles/we-had-the-vaccine-from-the-start-you-just-werent-allowed-to-take-it/
07:00 James Thompson, The Bitter Tiers of Mutant England, https://www.unz.com/jthompson/the-bitter-tiers-of-mutant-england/
13:00 Who Made the Vaccine Possible? Not WHO, https://www.wsj.com/articles/who-made-the-vaccine-possible-not-who-11608744603?mod=hp_opin_pos_1
23:00 One Day Sooner pushed for challenge trials, https://www.1daysooner.org/
25:00 Covid Vaccine: Should “Challenge Trials” Be Allowed?, https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=PlsCMGC60cY
35:30 Clinical Trials: From Design to FDA Consideration, https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9pfsYbMP47o
55:00 Generating self-compassion, https://www.tmswiki.org/ppd/TMS_Recovery_Program
1:19:00 What are we doing here?
2:20:00 Jared Taylor on BLM
2:21:30 Rush Limbaugh says goodbye
2:28:30 Let’s get energized

Philip Steele writes:

Few people realize that the Moderna vaccine against COVID-19—which the FDA has finally declared “highly effective,” and which is now being distributed to Americans—has actually been available for nearly a year.

But the government wouldn’t let you take it.

The vaccine, a triumph of medical science known as mRNA-1273, was designed in a single weekend, just two days after Chinese researchers published the virus’s genetic code on January 11, 2020.

For the entire duration of the pandemic, while hundreds of thousands died and the world economy was decimated by lockdowns, this highly effective vaccine has been available.

But you, and all the people who died, were prohibited by the government from taking it.

There are some who claim that the FDA “saves lives” by putting the brakes on medical innovation with their requirements for years-long, and often decades-long, billion-dollar medical trial procedures.

Missing here is the obvious counterpoint—How many lives did the FDA sacrifice to disease in the meantime?

In the case of COVID-19 we know the answer: more than 300,000 deaths so far in the United States and counting.

So why was this vaccine delayed for a full year? Because the FDA prohibited rapid “challenge trials”—where volunteers take the vaccine and then expose themselves to the virus in a lab, rather than waiting agonizing months to see how many catch the virus “in the wild.”

Challenge trials would have proven the vaccine’s effectiveness in a matter of weeks. But the FDA considered the risk to trial volunteers too high.

But why? Why are hundreds of thousands of “natural” deaths from a rampaging disease considered acceptable to the FDA—while the remote possibility of one or two deaths, in the absolute worst case scenario, among well-informed vaccine-testing volunteers are not?

There is no rational answer. The tragic truth is that we are ruled by a cowardly medical bureaucracy, one that would rather allow hundreds of thousands of people to die than face any potential criticism for allowing an accelerated vaccine trial.

By contrast, in a free society, immediately after the vaccine was created, volunteers would have been allowed to participate in challenge trials. The trials would have been conducted either by the vaccine company itself, or more likely by third-party medical-trial specialists, to remove any concerns about bias in the results.

The first small group of volunteers would be vaccinated, and then exposed to the virus. If the vaccine appeared to be safe and effective, then a larger group would be vaccinated.

As each challenge group proved successful, the number of volunteers for the next group would grow. Week by week the challenge groups would grow larger, until after just a few months—instead of taking nearly a year by the FDA’s “in the wild” method—the results would be definitive and the trials complete.

This means that in March or April of 2020—instead of the first wave of COVID deaths and lockdowns in the United States—we could have seen a wide vaccine rollout, leading to rapid herd immunity, nipping the pandemic in the bud.

But that path would have been possible only in a free society.

Instead, we have the FDA, backed by government force, dictating medical policy and drawing out the trial process for nearly a year, while death and economic destruction reign.

James Thompson writes for Unz.com: “the Moderna vaccine was available in January and could have been deployed from 24 February onwards. It took two days for a bright scientist to complete her mRNA design, and then it was ready to go into production.”

Posted in Covid | Comments Off on FEE: We Had the Vaccine from the Start—You Just Weren’t Allowed to Take It